Write a Reflection piece on your learning experiences

Write a Reflection piece on your learning experiences i.e. during the Study Skills module and writing the assignments. Please find the module information in the attached documents named as module information, topics areas, and Schedule. Also, find a ppt that shows how the module is organised.

Make sure the criteria in the question paper are implemented in written work. Will appreciate early work.

 

Order a Similar Paper

Must use annotated bibliography to best of ability

Must use annotated bibliography to best of ability (attached)

Write a 4–6-page research paper on the unique aerodynamic characteristics of swept wing aerodynamics.  Your paper should not discuss the capabilities of the aircraft unless these capabilities pertain to the aircraft’s unique aerodynamic design. The topics addressed in the Dole text provide specific aerodynamic areas to address as they relate to your chosen aircraft. Unique characteristics might include:

  • Airfoils and aerodynamic forces: unique airfoil design to enhance the airfoil’s purpose (speed, lift, etc.)
  • Lift/Drag: the relationship that lift and drag have to each other and the design characteristics to optimize each
  • Jet Aircraft/Propeller Aircraft: similarities and differences and their use in commercial/military/missions aviation
  • Slow-speed flight: how design enhances both performance and safety
  • Maneuvering Performance: maneuverability/controllability relationship and its effect on aircraft design
  • Longitudinal, directional, and lateral stability and control: the relationship between each during typical phases of flight
  • High-speed flight: those aspects unique to airframe, engine performance, and the challenges of flight from takeoff to landing

The paper must adhere to current APA formatting and include a title page, abstract, and reference page (none of which are included in the page count). A minimum of 3 sources must be used, only 2 of which can be from non-academic websites. Do not use Wiki sites. The other sources must be academic, scholarly resources.

 

Order a Similar Paper

Celebrity, Celebrity Culture and the effects on society

Issue/Topic: Celebrity, Celebrity Culture and the effects on society
1500 or more words
MLA format
Must include research from at least 4 scholarly sources (use HCC Library and GoogleScholar) I have attached 20 pdf with scholarly sources to choose from. 2 were provided from teacher Celebrity Culture Beneficial and The Culture of Celebrity. I have also attached a Word Document Research Paper Guide. Please read all the way to bottom more instructions at the bottom. Disregards Links and external cites those are the PDFs.

Celebrity is a popular cultural Links to an external site.phenomenon surrounding a well-known person. Though many celebritiesLinks to an external site. became famous as a result of their achievements or experiences, a person who obtains celebrity status does not necessarily need to have accomplished anything significant beyond being widely recognized by the public. Some celebrities use their fameLinks to an external site. to reach the upper levels of social status. Popular celebrities can wield significant influence over their fans and followers. Cultural historian and film critic Neal Gabler has described the phenomenon of celebrity as a process similar to performance art in which the celebrity builds intrigue and allure by presenting a manufactured image to the public. This image is reinforced through advertisingLinks to an external site. endorsements, appearances at high-profile events, tabloid gossip, and social mediaLinks to an external site. presence.

In previous decades, celebrity status was mainly reserved for film stars, televisionLinks to an external site. personalities, entertainersLinks to an external site., politicians, and athletesLinks to an external site.. Contemporary celebrities come from diverse fields ranging from astrophysics to auto mechanics, or they may simply be famous for their lifestyle or InternetLinks to an external site. antics. Social media platforms such as YouTube, Twitter, and Instagram provide the means for previously unknown individuals to cultivate a significant following. Celebrification is the process by which someone or something previously considered ordinary obtains stardom. Previously commonplace activities, such as practicing vegetarianismLinks to an external site. or wearing white t-shirts, can undergo celebrification when associated with a famous person or major event.

Celebrity culture exists when stardom becomes a pervasive part of the social order, commodified as a commercial brand. Celebrities’ personal lives are recast as products for consumption, with a dedicated fan base demanding information and unlimited access to the celebrity’s thoughts and activities. A niche community such as a fan base can be monetized through effective marketing that links brand loyalty to the consumer’s identity. Fans may be more likely to purchase a product or attend an event if they feel that doing so strengthens their relationship with a celebrity. Additionally, fans may worry that they will disappoint their idol if they do not own a specific item or support a specific cause. A person may feel that they need to support a celebrity if that support reinforces their membership in a specific group or demographic.

Find scholarly sources that focus on celebrity and celebrity culture and the impact it has had (whether past or present) and make arguable claims (take a position) on what these terms mean, how they have impacted society (positively? negatively? something in between?) and possibly how it is or has changed and what this means for the future. 

ALL QUOTATION OR PARAPHRASE should be cited in-paper (put the author’s name or other source identifier directly in your paper).  Failure to give credit to the author is plagiarism and will result in a zero for the

 

 

Order a Similar Paper

DR. BECKETT’S DENTAL OFFICE

DR. BECKETT’S DENTAL OFFICE

OVERVIEW

A dentist decides not to become a health maintenance organization (HMO) provider, because she feels like she cannot provide good dental care for her patients at the reimbursement rates provided by the HMOs. With the help of a consultant, she decides to differentiate her practice on the basis of quality. As the dentist’s patients may be charged fees that are not covered by their insurance policies, she must convince them that her superior quality offsets the additional financial costs. She constructs a new office building and redesigns her entire practice to communicate high quality to her patients, and to improve productivity through increased efficiency.

Questions –

1. Which of the seven elements of the services marketing mix are addressed in this case? Give examples of each “P” you identify?

2. Evaluate Dr. Beckett’s website (http://www.mychicodentist.com/). What strengths do you think the website has? What improvements would you suggest?

3. What supplementary services are offered? How do they enhance service delivery?

4.  Contrast your own dental care experiences with those offered by Dr. Beckett’s practice. What differences do you see? Based on your review of this case, what advice would you give (a) to your current or former dentist, and (b) to Dr. Beckett?

Write a minimum 5 page paper with your answers, using APA 7 Format and always list your references.

as well more two questions

1.  What are the steps involved in developing a service blueprint?

2.  List and describe the four tools that managers can use to guide their design of the service process.

 

 

Order a Similar Paper

Below are the Teaching Blocks and the corresponding area of study completed in each

Module MD4058 Study Skills

 

Below are the Teaching Blocks and the corresponding area of study completed in each

 

Teaching BlockTopic Area
1a. Introduction to the module

b. Locating and Evaluating Academic information & sources

2Academic Reading Strategies: Introduction to Critical Analysis/thinking and reading critically’
3Academic Integrity & Referencing Sources (in academic work and avoiding plagiarism by utilising quotations, paraphrasing, summarising)
4The Writing Process
5Academic Writing
6Self-Reflection and Academic Writing
7Writing for Assessment
Important Note:

· Please ensure that you attend both the lecture and seminar for each topic areas.

· There are also Pre lecture activities, Homework and Independent Study activities which need to be completed in addition to attending the lectures and seminars. These are self-study materials and to be completed in your own time.

· If you have questions about these at any time please contact your tutor via email or the TEAMs Chat function for an appointment

 

 

 

2 | Page

 

Order a Similar Paper

Reflection Questions

PLEASE ADD A PLAGIARISM CHECK

For this essay, you will select one of the articles provided below and engage in a 2-3 page summary and response dialogue with the source. This will involve providing a detailed summary of the source’s argument and responding to that argument with your position based on the information provided in the source. Articles will be attached in files.

A. Assignment Guidelines

DIRECTIONS: Refer to the list below throughout the writing process. Do not submit your Touchstone until it meets these guidelines.

1. Article Summary

❒ Have you communicated the source’s purpose?❒ Have you included all of the source’s main points?❒ Have you restated the source’s argument in your own words?

2. Article Response

❒ Have you provided your perspective on the source’s argument?❒ Have you used specific examples from the source to illustrate why you either agree or disagree with the argument?

3. Reflection

❒ Have you answered all reflection questions thoughtfully and included insights, observations, and/or examples in all responses? ❒ Are your answers included on a separate page below the main assignment?

B. Reflection Questions

DIRECTIONS: Below your assignment, include answers to all of the following reflection questions.

  1. What ideas originally came to mind when you first read through the article? Did your initial response to the article change after reading it for a second time? (3-4 sentences)
  2. How does paying attention to the way you respond to a source help you formulate your stance on a topic? (2-3 sentences)

D. Requirements

The following requirements must be met for your submission to be graded:

  • Composition must be 2-3 pages (approximately 500-750 words).
  • Double-space the composition and use one-inch margins.
  • Use a readable 12-point font.
  • All writing must be appropriate for an academic context.
  • Composition must be original and written for this assignment.
  • Plagiarism of any kind is strictly prohibited.
  • Submission must include your name, the name of the course, the date, and the title of your composition.
  • Include all of the assignment components in a single file.

Order a Similar Paper

construct an illustrated food web

Part 1: Your task for this project is to construct an illustrated food web to diagram trophic interactions in the Everglades ecosystem. The food web must be an original creation, you cannot submit a food web that you find online! You must use a minimum of 8 species that are found in the Everglades. You must indicate, using arrows or lines, the flow of energy between the species in your diagram. Be sure to upload your food web as an attachment in the assignment dropbox. Your constructed food web is worth 60 points.

Click here for a resource that will allow you to identify plants and animals that are found in the Everglades.

https://www.nps.gov/ever/learn/nature/index.htm

Part 2: Answer the following questions about the food web you constructed. Answers should be provided on a separate attachment in the assignment dropbox. Answers to the 10 questions using correct spelling and grammar are worth a total of 40 points.

1) List the producer(s) in your food web.

2) List the herbivores in your food web.

3) Are there any organisms in your food web that are omnivores? On which trophic levels are they feeding?

4) List the carnivores in your food web.

5) Identify and list a food chain within your food web that depicts at least three trophic levels. What organism in your selected food chain is a secondary consumer?

6) Are there any nonnative species in your food web? Briefly describe are they altering this food web in the Everglades ecosystem?

7) Choose a primary consumer in your food web. If its population suddenly started to decline, what density-dependent (biotic) factors could be causing it?

8) Choose a secondary consumer in your food web. If its population suddenly started to increase, what density-dependent  (biotic) factors could be causing it?

9) Are there any keystone species in your food web? If a keystone species were removed from your food web, how would its loss impact the other organisms?

10) Are there any endangered or threatened species in your food web? If the species goes extinct, how would its loss impact the other organisms?

 

Order a Similar Paper

The Cosmic Distance Ladder – Student Guide

Name:

NAAP – Cosmic Distance Ladder 1/7

The Cosmic Distance Ladder – Student Guide

Exercises The Cosmic Distance Ladder Module consists of material on seven different distance determination techniques. Four of the techniques have external simulators in addition to the background pages. You are encouraged to work through the material for each technique before moving on to the next technique.

Radar Ranging Question 1: Over the last 10 years, a large number of iceballs have been found in the

outer solar system out beyond Pluto. These objects are collectively known as the Kuiper

Belt. An amateur astronomer suggests using the radar ranging technique to learn the

rotation periods of Kuiper Belt Objects. Do you think that this plan would be

successful? Explain why or why not?

 

 

 

Parallax

In addition to astronomical applications, parallax is used for measuring distances in many other disciplines such as surveying. Open the Parallax Explorer where techniques very similar to those used by surveyors are applied to the problem of finding the distance to a boat out in the middle of a large lake by finding its position on a small scale drawing of the real world. The simulator consists of a map providing a scaled overhead view of the lake and a road along the bottom edge where our surveyor represented by a red X may be located. The surveyor is equipped with a theodolite (a combination of a small telescope and a large protractor so that the angle of the telescope orientation can be precisely measured) mounted on a tripod that can be moved along the road to establish a baseline. An Observer’s View panel shows the appearance of the boat relative to trees on the far shore through the theodolite.

Configure the simulator to preset A which allows us to see the location of the boat on the map. (This is a helpful simplification to help us get started with this technique – normally the main goal of the process is to learn the position of the boat on the scaled map.) Drag the position of the surveyor around and note how the apparent position of the boat relative to background objects changes. Position the surveyor to the far left of the road and click take measurement which causes the surveyor to sight the boat through the theodolite and measure the angle between the line of sight to the boat and the road. Now position the surveyor to the far right of the road and click take measurement again. The distance between these two positions defines the baseline of our observations and the intersection of the two red lines of sight indicates the position of the boat.

 

 

NAAP – Cosmic Distance Ladder 2/7

We now need to make a measurement on our scaled map and convert it back to a distance in the real world. Check show ruler and use this ruler to measure the distance from the baseline to the boat in an arbitrary unit. Then use the map scale factor to calculate the perpendicular distance from the baseline to the boat.

Question 2: Enter your perpendicular distance to the boat in map units. ______________

Show your calculation of the distance to the boat in meters in the box below.

 

 

 

 

 

 

Configure the simulator to preset B. The parallax explorer now assumes that our surveyor can make angular observations with a typical error of 3°. Due to this error we will now describe an area where the boat must be located as the overlap of two cones as opposed to a definite location that was the intersection of two lines. This preset is more realistic in that it does not illustrate the position of the boat on the map.

 

Question 3: Repeat the process of applying triangulation to determine the distance to the

boat and then answer the following:

What is your best estimate for the perpendicular distance to the boat?

 

What is the greatest distance to the boat that is still consistent with your observations?

 

What is the smallest distance to the boat that is still consistent with your observations?

 

 

Configure the simulator to preset C which limits the size of the baseline and has an error of 5° in each angular measurement.

Question 4: Repeat the process of applying triangulation to determine the distance to the

boat and then explain how accurately you can determine this distance and the factors

contributing to that accuracy.

 

 

 

 

NAAP – Cosmic Distance Ladder 3/7

 

 

Distance Modulus Question 5: Complete the following table concerning the distance modulus for several

objects.

Object Apparent

Magnitude m

Absolute Magnitude

M

Distance Modulus

m-M

Distance (pc)

Star A 2.4 10

Star B 5 16

Star C 10 25

Star D 8.5 0.5

 

Question 6: Could one of the stars listed in the table above be an RR Lyrae star? Explain

why or why not.

 

Spectroscopic Parallax Open up the Spectroscopic Parallax Simulator. There is a panel in the upper left entitled Absorption Line Intensities – this is where we can use information on the types of lines in a star’s spectrum to determine its spectral type. There is a panel in the lower right entitled Star Attributes where one can enter the luminosity class based upon information on the thickness of line in a star’s spectrum. This is enough information to position the star on the HR Diagram in the upper right and read off its absolute magnitude. Let’s work through an example. Imagine that an astronomer observes a star to have an apparent magnitude of 4.2 and collects a spectrum that has very strong helium and moderately strong ionized helium lines – all very thick. Find the distance to the star using spectroscopic parallax.

 

 

NAAP – Cosmic Distance Ladder 4/7

Let’s first find the spectral type. We can see in the Absorption Line Intensities panel that for the star to have any helium lines it must be a very hot blue star. By dragging the vertical cursor we can see that for the star to have very strong helium and moderate ionized helium lines it must either be O6 or O7. Since the spectral lines are all very thick, we can assume that it is a main sequence star. Setting the star to luminosity class V in the Star Attributes panel then determines its position on the HR Diagram and identifies its absolute magnitude as -4.1. We can complete the distance modulus calculation by setting the apparent magnitude slider to 4.2 in the Star Attributes panel. The distance modulus is 8.3 corresponding to a distance of 449 pc. Students should keep in mind that spectroscopic parallax is not a particularly precise technique even for professional astronomers. In reality, the luminosity classes are much wider than they are shown in this simulation and distances determined by this technique are probably have uncertainties of about 20%. Question 7: Complete the table below by applying the technique of spectroscopic

parallax.

Observational Data Analysis

m Description of spectral lines Description of line thickness M m-M d

(pc)

6.2 strong hydrogen lines moderate helium lines very thin

13.1 strong molecular lines very thick

7.2 strong ionized metal lines moderate hydrogen lines very thick

 

 

 

 

 

NAAP – Cosmic Distance Ladder 5/7

Main Sequence Fitting Open up the Cluster Fitting Explorer. Note that the main sequence data for nearby stars whose distances are known are plotted by absolute magnitude in red on the HR Diagram. In the Cluster Selection Panel, choose the Pleiades cluster. The Pleiades data are then added in apparent magnitude in blue. Note that the two y-axes are aligned, but the two main sequences don’t overlap due to the distance of the Pleiades (since it is not 10 parsecs away).

If you move the cursor into the HR diagram, the cursor will change to a handle, and you can shift the apparent magnitude scale by clicking and dragging. Grab the cluster data and drag it until the two main sequences are best overlapped as shown to the right.

We can now relate the two y-axes. Check show horizontal bar which will automate the process of determining the offset between the m and M axes. Note that it doesn’t matter where you compare the m and M values, at all points they will give the proper distance modulus. One set of values gives m – M = 1.6 – (-4.0) = 5.6 which corresponds to a distance of 132 pc.

Question 8: Note that there are several stars that are above the main sequence in the

upper left. Can you explain why these stars are not on the main sequence?

 

 

 

Question 9: Note that there are several stars below the main sequence especially near

temperatures of about 5000K. Can you explain why these stars are not on the main

sequence?

 

 

 

 

 

 

NAAP – Cosmic Distance Ladder 6/7

Question 10: Determine the distance to the Hyades cluster.

 

Apparent magnitude m Absolute Magnitude M Distance (pc)

 

 

Question 11: Determine the distance to the M67 cluster.

 

Apparent magnitude m Absolute Magnitude M Distance (pc)

 

Cepheids Question 12: A type II Cepheid has an apparent magnitude of 12 and a pulsation period

of 3 days. Determine the distance to the Cepheid variable and explain your method in the

box below?

 

 

 

 

 

Supernovae Open up the Supernovae Light Curve Explorer. It functions similarly to the Cluster Fitting Explorer. The red line illustrates the expected profile for a Type I supernovae in terms of Absolute Magnitude. Data from various supernovae can be graphed in terms of apparent magnitude. If the data represents a Type I Supernovae it should be possible to fit the data to the Type I profile with the appropriate shifts in time and magnitude. Once the data fit the profile, then the difference between apparent and absolute magnitude again gives the distance modulus. As an example load the data for 1995D. Grab and drag the data until it best matches the Type I profile as shown. One can then use the show horizontal bar option to help calculate the distance modulus. One pair of values is m – M = 13-(-20) = 33 which corresponds to a distance of 40 Mpc.

 

 

 

NAAP – Cosmic Distance Ladder 7/7

 

Question 13: Determine the distance to Supernovae 1994ae and explain your method in

the box below?

 

 

 

 

 

Question 14: Load the data for Supernova 1987A. Explain why it is not possible to

determine the distance to this supernova?

 

 

 

 

 

 

  • The Cosmic Distance Ladder – Student Guide
  • Exercises
    • Radar Ranging
    • Parallax
    • Distance Modulus
    • Main Sequence Fitting
    • Cepheids

 

Order a Similar Paper

 Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois

 Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois

The Recess Debate A Disjuncture between Educational Policy

and Scientific Research •

Anthony D. Pellegrini

Some devalue recess because they assume it to be a waste of time. There is no theory or empirical evidence to support this point of view. There is, however, abundant and clear evidence that recess has beneficial effects on children’s social competence and academic performance. The author tells how his interest in standardized tests led him to years of recess study, compares recess survey findings in the United States to those in the United Kingdom, and summarizes the benefits of recess for school performance.

Recess has been part of the school day for as long as we can remem- ber. Typically, most people have considered what children do during recess as merely “playful.” Adults usually regard it as a break from the serious work of the day—reading, writing, and arithmetic—while kids often say, perhaps only half-jokingly, that it is their favorite time. Because what goes on at recess does not appear serious, some claim it interferes with the “educational” mis- sion of schools. This perception has led many districts to question the need for recess. Since I explored this trend in considerable detail three years ago in Recess: Its Role in Education and Development, recess has remained under attack in both the United States and the United Kingdom. The debate over recess began around the same time (the early 1980s) in both countries and revolves around similar issues in both places. The onslaughts against recess persist today, even in the face of significant research supporting its educational value, a lack of research supporting a contrary view, and a rising awareness of the importance of play in general. Thus, it is useful to look anew at the arguments for and against recess and to be reminded of what the evidence does and does not show.

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 181 9/8/08 4:07:23 PM

 

 

182 A m E R I C A N J O u R N A L O F P L A Y   •   F a l l 2 0 0 8

The Argument against Recess

Breaks during the school day, like breaks from work on the factory assembly lines, have existed for nearly as long as each of those institutions has existed. Indeed, the rationale for breaks in both is very similar: after a reasonable amount of work, you need a break, if for no other reason than it may help you to be more productive. If you have never worked on an assembly line or do not remember your primary school days, perhaps you can remember driving on a long trip. You probably recall that the longer you drove the less attentive (and less safe) you became. If you pulled over for a rest or a break, you were more attentive (and safer) after you started again. This explains why many states have laws governing the length of time truckers and airline pilots can drive or fly without a break. This rather simple but powerful and widely understood benefit of breaks has not deterred a group, generally comprised of school administrators, from reducing recess time or eliminating recess all together from the school day. The reasons these “no nonsense” school superintendents and principals, as well as many politicians, most often give are twofold. First, they claim that recess is a waste of valuable time that could be more profitably used for instruction. Sec- ond, they claim that during recess kids get bullied and that on the playground they learn aggression. Politicians and school administrators often use the first argument—recess is a waste of instructional time—to demonstrate that they “mean business” in making schools more effective. A number of years ago, then Atlanta Public Schools superintendent Benjamin Canada and I discussed the role of recess in schools on the Good Morning America TV show. I was touted as the “expert” on recess, whereas Canada had made national news for proudly eliminating recess in Atlanta schools and replacing it with physical education. He claimed that by eliminating recess from the whole school system he had raised achievement scores. Recess, he said, was a waste of time, and kids did not learn by “hanging on monkey bars.” They could just as easily “blow off steam” in physical education while at the same time learning useful skills. When pressed by both me and the TV host for evidence of how achievement had gone up as a result of eliminating recess, Canada did not provide supporting data, and to my knowledge no one has ever presented data to uphold such a claim. The evidence is exactly the opposite of Canada’s claims. As I shall summa- rize below, in numerous controlled experiments children’s attention to school

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 182 9/8/08 4:07:24 PM

 

 

tasks decreased the longer they were deprived of a break and, correspondingly, children were significantly more attentive after recess than before. It is very much like taking a break on a long highway trip. Contrary to popular belief, physical education classes do not provide such a benefit. In 2001, the Council on Physical Education for Children, a national organization of physical education teachers, denounced the idea of replacing recess with physical education, although the council had a vested interested in promoting physical education. As the council members would surely agree, physical education—like other instructional disciplines—rightfully imposes rigorous demands on children and adolescents so as to stretch their skills. Therefore, it seems clear, the demands of a physical education class do not constitute a break. The second argument—that during recess, especially playground recess, kids get bullied—also has flaws. It is true that kids get bullied on playgrounds, but they get bullied in cafeterias, too, and in hallways, in bathrooms, in locker rooms, just about anywhere with little or no adult supervision. Even so, the base rate of aggression on playgrounds is incredibly low. Specifically, of all the behaviors observed on preschool and primary school playgrounds in many countries, physical and verbal aggression account for less than 2 percent of the total (Pellegrini 1995; Smith and Connolly 1980). The fact that rates of aggression are low at recess does not mean there are no incidents that damage kids. Aggressive behavior can be intense even when its rates of occurrence are low, and where there is intense aggression, people get hurt. However, adult supervision of recess periods, like adult supervision of the cafeteria and the hallways between classes, has a potent effect on dampening aggression (Pellegrini 2002). Contrary to the negative-behavior argument, recess remains one of the only times during the school day when children have time and opportunities to interact with their peers on their own terms. Through interaction at recess, children learn social skills, such as how to cooperate and compromise and how to inhibit ag- gression. Eliminating or reducing recess destroys these learning opportunities.

Why Study Recess? One Researcher’s Journey

Before examining the research in favor of recess, I should note how I came to it. As an academic psychologist, I should be concerned with the ways in

T h e R e c e s s D e b a t e 183

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 183 9/8/08 4:07:24 PM

 

 

184 A m E R I C A N J O u R N A L O F P L A Y   •   F a l l 2 0 0 8

which children learn and develop in school. Studying recess and how children become socially competent seems a more legitimate venue for an educational psychologist. However, having investigated the role of children’s play in their social and cognitive development for many years, especially play fighting (Pel- legrini and Smith 1998; Pellegrini 2002, 2003) and the games of boys and girls on school playgrounds (Pellegrini et al. 2002), the study of recess seemed a logical extension. My interest in school recess was really piqued by the debate over the role of recess in Georgia in the early 1990s (well before Benjamin Canada’s claims on Good Morning America) and the simultaneous use of standardized tests as the sole criterion for the promotion of children from kindergarten to first grade. As part of this—in my view, very questionable—venture, there was talk of eliminating recess so kids could spend more time on the “important skills” necessary to pass the tests. The argument went like this: test scores are declining, and so given the limited number of hours in the school day, it makes sense to eliminate or minimize a practice that is trivial at best and, in any case, antitheti- cal to more serious educational enterprise. My first reaction to the testing question was disbelief. We have known for decades that kindergarteners are unreliable test takers (Messick 1983). Kids tend not to perform consistently across time. For example, they could score in the 99th percentile on Tuesday, but if they retook the very same test on Wednesday, they could score in the 65th percentile. If they took it a third time on Thursday, they could score in the 99th percentile again. The different scores could be due to something as simple as a swing in motivation related to a change in the testing environment. (I observed this particular example in my own daughter’s experience.) Because children are unreliable test takers, it is important for educators to use a number of different assessment strategies. Tests can and should be used, but in conjunction with other measures, such as attendance, grades, teacher evaluations, and observations of behavioral competence. When all of these things are aggregated, we get a more valid picture (Cronbach 1971). When the testing question arose in Georgia, I had been studying rough and tumble play on the school playground during recess for several years. As part of this research I had access to test scores from kindergarten through at least first grade. I knew that what kids did on the playground required pretty high levels of social cognitive competence, and I knew that kids were motivated to implement those skills on the playground because they enjoyed interacting with

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 184 9/8/08 4:07:24 PM

 

 

their peers. So I wondered if what kindergarten children did on the playground could be a valid predictor of their first-grade achievement, as measured by a standardized test. That is, does kindergarten playground behavior predict first- grade test scores, even after we control statistically for academic achievement in kindergarten? In essence, I wanted to know if there was predictive academic value in what kindergarteners did at recess, beyond that information provided in their kindergarten academic achievement, as measured by a standardized test score. How much did recess activities tell us, beyond test scores, about how well kindergarteners would do in first grade? My hypothesis was that the recess behavior would tell us a great deal. After all, when kids are on the playground they are typically interacting with their peers, and to do so takes some pretty sophisticated skills. For example, to play cooperatively with their peers, children have to be able and willing to see things from the perspectives of their peers, use compromise to resolve conflicts, follow the rules of play and games, and use language to negotiate all of this. Indeed, we know that the types of language kids use to negotiate conflicts and compromise are very similar to the language of school instruction (Heath 1983) and the language of literacy (Pellegrini and Galda 1982). Further, when kids manipulate and build with playground materials and when they play games—such as tag—with their peers, they are motivated to marshal their social cognitive resources. Children generally like to interact with their peers at recess, so they try their best to initiate and sustain play. For instance, one may have to compromise (share a toy or a turn) in order to continue to play with one’s best friend. One typically does this because one is motivated to do so, perhaps more so than to perform on an achievement test. Tests, at least for most young kids, are not very motivating. These kindergarten behavioral measures that I developed and adminis- tered did indeed predict first-grade achievement, beyond the kindergarten test scores. That is, these playground behaviors were correlated with first-grade test scores, even after kindergarten test scores were statistically controlled. This reinforces the notion that multiple measures should be used in “high-stakes” assessments. In an effort to change policy in the state of Georgia, my friend and colleague Carl Glickman and I wrote articles for such publications as the Atlanta Journal Constitution and Principal to publicize our finding to the general public and educators of young children. Afterward, testing policies changed in Georgia, but efforts to minimize or eliminate recess continued to grow, both in the United

T h e R e c e s s D e b a t e 185

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 185 9/8/08 4:07:24 PM

 

 

186 A m E R I C A N J O u R N A L O F P L A Y   •   F a l l 2 0 0 8

States and the United Kingdom, where I was also conducting research. Policy makers, teachers, parents, newspapers, and radio and television stations in both countries began contacting me and asking about recess.

The Reduction of Recess in the united States and the united Kingdom

An important barometer of prevailing perceptions of the importance of recess is the way in which recess time has eroded across the last fifteen years. One of the first surveys of recess in the United States was conducted in 1989 by the National Association of Elementary School Principals (NAESP), which kindly sent me their findings. The survey went to school superintendents in all fifty states and the District of Columbia. Responses were received from forty-seven states and showed that recess existed, in some form, in 90 percent of all school districts. Generally, individual schools (87 percent of those reporting) set re- cess policy. Consequently, there was significant variation both within school districts and within states. Ninety-six percent of the schools with recess had it once or twice per day. In 75 percent of the schools with recess, it lasted fifteen to twenty minutes. The survey did not report what form that recess took or whether organized physical education was counted as recess. Indeed, about one-half of the districts with recess had “structured” times. Regarding recess supervision, the survey indicated that teachers assumed responsibility in 50 percent of the cases and teachers’ aides in 36 percent. Among the aides, 86 percent had no formal training for supervision. This is not a trivial finding. A well-trained supervisor can both support the positive social interac- tions of children and guard against aggression and bullying. Ten years later, the U.S. Department of Education surveyed recess in kin- dergarten. According to a summary provided to the author by Ithel Jones, As- sociate Professor of Early Childhood Education at Florida State University, 71 percent of surveyed kindergartens reported having a daily recess period; 14.6 percent had recess three to four times per week; 6.7 percent had recess one to two times per week; and 7.7 percent had no recess. Regarding the duration of recess, 27 percent had thirty minutes; 67 percent had sixteen to thirty minutes; and 6 percent had less than fifteen minutes. Children attending private kinder- gartens were twice as likely to have recess as children attending public schools: 48.3 percent vs. 22.2 percent.

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 186 9/8/08 4:07:24 PM

 

 

While a direct comparison with the 1989 survey is not possible, there are some interesting points to consider. Most interesting is that in kindergarten only 70 percent of the children had daily recess. If there is one grade where we would assume that all children would have recess daily, it would be kindergarten. In the late 1990s, British psychologist Peter Blatchford and colleagues (Blatch- ford and Sumpner 1998) conducted a national survey of recess (called “break time” in England) in primary and secondary schools across the United Kingdom. Their 60 percent return rate produced a sample of 6 percent of all English schools. Importantly, recess in the United Kingdom is uniform compared to recess in the United States. In the United Kingdom, schools have a morning, lunch, and after- noon break. The Blatchford survey showed that while students across all grades had breaks, the duration decreased with age. Children in infant school (five to seven years of age) had ninety-three minutes; children in junior school (seven to eleven years of age) had eighty-three minutes; and children in secondary school (eleven to sixteen years of age) had seventy-seven minutes. Clearly, English chil- dren had much more recess than their American counterparts, and the duration of the periods seemed more sensitive to the maturity of the students. There is, however, a movement against recess in the United Kingdom as well. The issues propelling this movement are very similar to those in the United States and have been very evident in the media. There, too, pressure has resulted in a reduction in break time. Within the five-year period from 1990–1991 to 1995–1996, 38 percent and 35 percent, respectively, of junior and secondary schools reduced the lunch break. Among infant schools, 26 percent reduced the lunch break and 12 percent eliminated the afternoon break. Twenty-seven percent of the junior schools and 14 percent of the secondary schools eliminated the afternoon break. One would think that such drastic change should be directed by empirical support, but, no, on the contrary, research supports keeping recess in schools.

Benefits of Recess for School Performance

There are two main arguments for the continued presence of recess in pri- mary schools. The first is evidence of how learning benefits from “distributed practice” (like the example of taking a break during highway driving noted earlier), which recess affords. The second concerns the development of cogni- tive efficiency and how recess may especially facilitate learning in younger and

T h e R e c e s s D e b a t e 187

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 187 9/8/08 4:07:25 PM

 

 

188 A m E R I C A N J O u R N A L O F P L A Y   •   F a l l 2 0 0 8

cognitively immature children. Both of these arguments propose that benefits associated with recess are immediate, that is, they occur almost simultaneously with the recess behaviors themselves.

Massed vs. distributed practice We have known for many years (e.g., Ebinghaus 1885; James 1901) that children learn better and more quickly when their efforts toward a task are distributed rather than concentrated or when they are given breaks during tasks (Hunter 1929). As psychologist Frank Dempster pointed out (1988), the positive effects of distributed effort have been seen specifically in the ways children learn how to conduct numerous school-like tasks, such as mastering native- and foreign- language vocabularies, text materials, and math facts. Laboratory studies have yielded reliable and robust findings, documenting the efficacy of task spacing on learning. Indeed, the theory has been supported by research with humans across the life span and with a variety of other animals. Classroom studies have been less frequent, and generally the results less supportive of the theory. Factors associated with the nature of a task (e.g., simple vs. complex) seem to influence the effects of distributed practice on classroom learning. However, when the nature of the criterion variable is changed from material learned to attention to the task at hand, the results of the classroom research match those of the laboratory. Spacing of tasks may make them less boring and correspondingly facilitate attention. Attention to a task, in turn, may be important to subsequent learning (Dempster 1988). Given the positive effects of distributed practice on children’s attention to school tasks, it seems puzzling that it has not been more readily used in class- rooms. One possibility, as suggested by Dempster (1988), is that the complicated contingencies of running a school may not readily accommodate the added complexities of a distributed practice regimen. The solution to this conundrum is simple—use a well-established school institution, recess. Recess provides a break between school tasks, thus distributing practice.

Developmental differences in cognitive efficiency Psychologist David F. Bjorklund and I have suggested previously (Pellegrini and Bjorklund 1997), based on Bjorklund’s theory of “cognitive immaturity” (Bjorklund and Green 1992), that the facilitative effects of breaks between peri- ods of intense work should be greater for younger than for older children. From our position, young children do not process most information as effectively

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 188 9/8/08 4:07:25 PM

 

 

as older children. The immaturity of their nervous systems and their lack of experiences render them unable to perform higher-level cognitive tasks with the same efficiency as older children and adults, and this directly influences their educability. As a result, young children are especially susceptible to the effects of interference and should experience the greatest gains from breaks between focused intellectual activities, which recess provides. Evidence in support of this hypothesis can be found in the literature on memory and cognitive inhibition. Research using a wide range of tasks has shown that children are increasingly able, as they get older, to inhibit task-ir- relevant thoughts and to resist interference from task-irrelevant stimuli, and that such skills contribute significantly to overall cognitive functioning (e.g., Bjorklund and Harnishfeger 1990). Inhibition abilities have been proposed to play a significant role in attention, permitting children to focus on task-relevant information and not to be distracted by task-irrelevant, peripheral information. Such abilities have also been proposed to be of central importance to functional working-memory capacity. Young children have a difficult time keeping extra- neous information from entering short-term store. As a result, their working memories are often cluttered with irrelevant information, leaving less mental space for task-relevant information or for the execution of cognitive strategies (Bjorklund and Harnishfeger 1990). From this perspective, there may be a general increase in interference when children perform a series of highly focused tasks, regardless of the nature of those tasks. Although one would predict that changing from one type of focused activity to another would yield some cognitive benefit, children (especially young children) may experience a continued buildup of interference with re- peated performance of even different highly focused tasks, and thus experi- ence greater benefit from a drastic change in activity, such as is afforded by recess. This is consistent with the evidence that younger children may require a greater change in activity or stimulus materials before they experience a re- lease from interference (e.g., Pellegrini and Bjorklund 1996). This should make school learning particularly difficult for young elementary school children, and opportunities to engage in non-focused, nonintellectual activities should af- ford them the needed respite to re-energize their nervous systems so that they can continue to learn in school. Consistent with this reasoning, recess periods across the school day should minimize cognitive interference. Importantly, instructional regimens, such as physical education, would not serve the same purpose.

T h e R e c e s s D e b a t e 189

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 189 9/8/08 4:07:25 PM

 

 

190 A m E R I C A N J O u R N A L O F P L A Y   •   F a l l 2 0 0 8

Conclusion

Some devalue recess because they assume it to be—as they assume play in young children to be—a waste of time, time that could be otherwise more ef- ficiently spent. There is no theory or empirical evidence to support this point of view. The counter-argument, that recess is good, is backed by a large body of theory and empirical research. Those who advocate the elimination of recess should present sound theoretical and empirical support for their arguments or give them up and recognize the abundant and clear evidence that recess has beneficial effects on children’s social competence and academic perfor- mance.

References

Bjorklund, David F. 1978. Negative transfer in children’s recall of categorized materials. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 26:299–307.

———. 2004, first published 1989. Children’s thinking: Developmental function and individual differences. 4th ed.

Bjorklund, David F., and Brandi L. Green. 1992. The adaptive nature of cognitive im- maturity. American Psychologist 47:46–54.

Bjorklund, David F., and Katherine K. Harnishfeger. 1990. The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review 10:48–71.

Bjorklund, David F., and Anthony D. Pellegrini. 2002. The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology.

Blatchford, Peter, and Clare Sumpner. 1998. What do we know about breaktime? Re- sults from a national survey of breaktime and lunchtime in primary and secondary schools. British Educational Research Journal 24:79–94.

Cronbach, Lee J. 1971. Test validation. In Educational measurement, ed. Robert L. Thorndike, 443–507.

Dempster, Frank N. 1988. The spacing effect. American Psychologist 43:627–34. Ebbinghaus, Hermann. 1964, first published 1885. Memory: A contribution to experi-

mental psychology. Heath, Shirley. 1983. Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and

classrooms. Hunter, Walter S. 1929. Learning II: Experimental studies of learning. In The founda-

tions of experimental psychology, ed. Carl Murchison, 564–627. James, William. 1901. Talks to teachers on psychology, and to students on some of life’s

ideals.

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 190 9/8/08 4:07:25 PM

 

 

Messick, Sam. 1983. Assessment of children. In Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 1, History, theory, and methods, ed. William Kessen, 477–526.

Pellegrini, Anthony D. 1990. Elementary school children’s playground behavior: Im- plications for children’s social-cognitive development. Children’s Environments Quarterly 7:8–16.

———. 1991. Outdoor recess: Is it really necessary? Principal 70:40. ———. 1992. Kindergarten children’s social-cognitive status as a predictor of first-grade

success. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 7:565–77. ———. 1995. School recess and playground behavior: Educational and developmental

roles. ———. 2002. Bullying, victimization, and sexual harassment during the transition to

middle school. Educational Psychologist 37:151–63. ———. 2003. Perceptions and functions of play and real fighting in early adolescence.

Child Development 74:1522–33. ———. 2005. Recess: Its role in education and development. Pellegrini, Anthony D., and David F. Bjorklund. 1996. The place of recess in school:

Issues in the role of recess in children’s education and development: An introduc- tion to the theme of the Special Issue. Journal of Research in Childhood Education 11:5–13.

———. 1997. The role of recess in children’s cognitive performance. Educational Psy- chologist 32:35–40.

Pellegrini, Anthony D., and Lee Galda. 1982. The effects of thematic-fantasy play train- ing on the development of children’s story comprehension. American Educational Research Journal 19:443–52.

Pellegrini, Anthony D., Kentaro Kato, Peter Blatchford, and Ed Baines. 2002. A short- term longitudinal study of children’s playground games across the first year of school: Implications for social competence and adjustment to school. American Educational Research Journal 39:991–1015.

Pellegrini, Anthony D., and Peter K. Smith. 1998. Physical activity play: The nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Development 69:577–98.

Smith, Peter K., and Kevin Connolly. 1980. The ecology of preschool behavior.

T h e R e c e s s D e b a t e 191

AMJP 01_2 text.indd 191 9/8/08 4:07:26 PM

 

Order a Similar Paper

TFE requirement

I have had a few questions from students about the week three requirement.  Here is some more information to help you complete the TFE requirement.  Make sure you attach your excel spreadsheet showing your work to your analysis.  Are you familiar with the capability of excel to complete calculations in the cells? To activate the function, use the equal sign ( = ) in the cell where you want to complete the equation.  When you type the equal sign in the cell, excel will calculate the answer for any equation you enter.  Start with something easy: Say you have data value 10 in cell C4 and 15 in cell C5.  To add the cells, you choose another cell (D6), and enter =C4+C5.  Hit enter and the answer 25 shows in the cell.  When you highlight the cell again, you can see the equation in the function bar at the top of the page.

You can do this with any math function [add, subtract, multiply, divide ] ( + – * / ).  Excel also has advanced functions like: square root [ =sqrt( ) ] Mean [ =average( )  ]

 

 

For this problem, use formulas 8.40, 8.41, and 8.42, to build up to completing the upper and lower control limit [formula 8.43]. For the standard deviation, start at the left side of the equation and break it down into components  [square root, multiply, subtraction, and divide].  Enter those in the excel cells to arrive at the final answer.

Therefore, upper and lower control limits are given by

I know the excel template for this problem is difficult to find and download, so I attached my copy of the excel template to this message.    Reference: Evans, J. R., & Lindsay, W. M. (2017). Managing for quality and performance excellence (10th ed.). Mason, OH: South-Western Cengage.

 

Order a Similar Paper