Lexeme formation: the familiar 39
Lexeme formation: the familiar 39
3. Some speakers will fi nd the forms in (3b) odd, and will question their acceptability, but they are all attested
in the Corpus of Contemporary American English and discussed in Bauer, Lieber, and Plag ( 2013 ).
goes on. This morpheme (an a for the verb ‘love’ and an i for the verb ‘say’)
doesn’t mean anything, but still must be added before the inflectional
ending can be attached. The root plus this extra morpheme is the stem .
Thought of another way, the stem is usually the base that is left when the
inflectional endings are removed. We will look further at roots and stems
in Chapter 6 , when we discuss inflection more fully.
3.3 Affixation
3.3.1 Word formation rules Let’s look more carefully at words derived by affixation . Prefixes and suf-
fixes usually have special requirements for the sorts of bases they can
attach to. Some of these requirements concern the phonology (sounds) of
their bases, and others concern the semantics (meaning) of their bases –
we will return to these shortly – but the most basic requirements are often
the syntactic part of speech or category of their bases. For example, the
suffix -ness attaches freely to adjectives, as the examples in (3a) show and
sometimes to nouns (as in (3b) ), but not to verbs (3c) :
(3) a. – ness on adjectives: redness, happiness, wholeness, commonness,
niceness
b. – ness on nouns: appleness, babeness, couch-potatoness 3
c. – ness on verbs: *runness, *wiggleness, *yawnness
The prefix un- attaches to adjectives (where it means ‘not’) and to verbs
(where it means ‘reverse action’), but not to nouns:
(4) a. un – on adjectives: unhappy, uncommon, unkind, unserious
b. un- on verbs: untie, untwist, undress, unsnap
c. un- on nouns: *unchair, *unidea, *ungiraffe
We might begin to build some of the rules that native speakers of English use
for making words with -ness or un- by stating their categorial requirements:
(5) Rule for -ness (first version): Attach – ness to an adjective or to a noun.
Rule for un- (first version): Attach un- to an adjective or to a verb.
Of course, if we want to be as precise as possible about what native speak-
ers know about forming words with these affixes, we should also indicate
what category of word results from using these affixes, and what the
resulting word means. So a more complete version of our – ness and un-
rules might look like (6) :
(6) Rule for – ness (second version): – ness attaches to adjectives or nouns
‘X’ and produces nouns meaning ‘the quality of X’.
Rule for un- (second version): un- attaches to adjectives meaning ‘X’
and produces adjectives meaning ‘not X’; un – attaches to verbs
meaning ‘X’ and produces verbs meaning ‘reverse the action X’.
40 INTRODUCING MORPHOLOGY
If we’re really trying to model what native speakers of English know about
these affixes, we might try to be even more precise. For example, un- does
not attach to all adjectives or verbs, as you can discover by looking at the
next Challenge box.
Challenge
Look at the following words and try to work out more details of the
rule for un- in English. The (a) list contains some adjectives to which
negative un- can be attached and others which seem impossible or at
least somewhat odd. The (b) list contains some verbs to which
reversative un- can attach and others which seem impossible. See if
you can discern some patterns:
(a) unhappy, *unsad, unlovely, *unugly, unintelligent, *unstupid
(b) untie, unwind, unhinge, unknot, *undance, *unyawn, *unexplode,
*unpush
What the (a) examples in the Challenge box seem to show is that the negative
prefix un- in English prefers to attach to bases that do not themselves have
negative connotations. This is not true all of the time – adjectives like unself-
ish or unhostile are attested in English – but it’s at least a significant tendency.
As for the (b) examples, they suggest that the un- that attaches to verbs pre-
fers verbal bases that imply some sort of result, and moreover that the result
is not permanent. Verbs like dance, push, and yawn denote actions that have
no results, and although explode implies a result (i.e., something is blown up),
it’s a result that is permanent. In contrast, a verb like tie implies a result
(something is in a bow or knot) which is temporary (you can take it apart).
We have just constructed what morphologists call a word formation
rule , a rule which makes explicit all the categorial, semantic, and phono-
logical information that native speakers know about the kind of base that
an affix attaches to and about the kind of word it creates. We might now
state the full word formation rules for negative un- as in (7) :
(7) Rule for negative un- (final version): un- attaches to adjectives,
preferably those with neutral or positive connotations, and creates
negative adjectives. It has no phonological restrictions.
Now let’s look at two more affixes. In English we can form new verbs by
using the suffixes -ize or -ify . Both of these suffixes attach to either nouns
or adjectives, resulting in verbs:
(8) -ize on adjectives: civilize, idealize, finalize, romanticize,
tranquillize
-ize on nouns: unionize, crystallize, hospitalize, caramelize,
animalize
-ify on adjectives: purify, glorify, uglify, moistify, diversify
– ify on nouns: mummify, speechify, classify, brutify, scarify,
bourgeoisify
Lexeme formation: the familiar 41
We might state the word formation rules for -ize and -ify as in (9) :
(9) Rule for -ize (first version): -ize attaches to adjectives or nouns that
mean ‘X’ and produces verbs that mean ‘make/put into X’.
Rule for -ify (first version): -ify attaches to adjectives or nouns that
mean ‘X’ and produces verbs that mean ‘make/put into X’.
But again, we can be a bit more precise about these rules. Although -ize
and -ify have almost identical requirements for the category of base they
attach to and produce words with roughly the same meaning, they have
somewhat different requirements on the phonological form of the stem
they attach to. As the examples in (8) show, -ize prefers words with two or
more syllables where the final syllable doesn’t bear primary stress (e.g.,
trá nquil, hó spital ). The suffix -ify , on the other hand, prefers monosyllabic
bases ( pure, brute, scar ), although it also attaches to bases that end in a – y
( mummy, ugly ) or bases whose final syllables are stressed ( divé rse, bourgé ois ).
Since we want to be as precise as possible about our word formation rules
for these suffixes, we will state their phonological restrictions along with
their categorial needs:
(10) Rule for -ize (final version): -ize attaches to adjectives or nouns of
two or more syllables where the final syllable does not bear
primary stress. For a base ‘X’ it produces verbs that mean ‘make/put
into X’.
I leave it to you to come up with the final version of the word formation
rule for – ify.
3.3.2 Word structure When you divide up a complex word into its morphemes, as in (11) , it’s
easy to get the impression that words are put together like the beads that
make up a necklace – one after the other in a line:
(11) unhappiness = un + happy + ness
But morphologists believe that words are more like onions than like neck-
laces: onions are made up of layers from innermost to outermost. Consider
a word like unhappiness . We can break this down into its component mor-
phemes un + happy + ness , but given what we learned above about the
properties of the prefix un- and the suffix – ness we know something more
about the way in which this word is constructed beyond just its constitu-
ent parts. We know that un- must first go on the base happy . Happy is an
adjective, and un- attaches to adjectives but does not change their catego-
ry. The suffix -ness attaches only to adjectives and makes them into nouns.
So if un- attaches first to happy and -ness attaches next, the requirements
of both affixes are met. But if we were to do it the other way around, -ness
would have first created a noun, and then un- would be unable to attach.
We could represent the order of attachment as if words really were
onions, with the base in the innermost layer, and each affix in its own
succeeding layer: see Figure 3.2 .
42 INTRODUCING MORPHOLOGY
But linguists, not generally being particularly artistic, prefer to show
these relationships as ‘trees’ that look like this :
(12)
N
A
A
un happy ness
Similarly, we might represent the structure of a word like repurify as in (13) :
(13)
V
V
A
re pure ify
In order to draw this structure, we must first know that the prefix re-
attaches to verbs (e.g., reheat, rewash , or redo ) but not to adjectives ( *repure,
*rehappy ) or to nouns ( *rechair, *retruth ). Once we know this, we can say that
the adjective pure must first be made into a verb by suffixing -ify , and only
then can re- attach to it.
FIGURE 3.2 Words are like onions
Challenge
In English, the suffix -ize attaches to nouns or adjectives to form
verbs. The suffix -ation attaches to verbs to form nouns. And the suffix
-al attaches to nouns to form adjectives. Interestingly, these suffixes
Lexeme formation: the familiar 43
3.3.3 What do affixes mean? When we made the distinction between affixes and bound bases above, we
did so on the basis of a rather vague notion of semantic robustness; bound
bases in some sense had more meat to them than affixes did. Let us now
attempt to make that idea a bit more precise by looking at typical mean-
ings of affixes.
In some cases, affixes seem to have not much meaning at all. Consider
the suffixes in (14) :
(14) a. -(a)tion examination, taxation, realization, construction
– ment agreement, placement, advancement, postponement
-al refusal, arousal, disposal
b. -ity purity, density, diversity, complexity
-ness happiness, thickness, rudeness, sadness
Beyond turning verbs into nouns with meanings like ‘process of X-ing’ or
‘result of X-ing’, where X is the meaning of the verb, it’s not clear that the
suffixes -(a)tion, -ment , and -al add much of any meaning at all. Similarly
with -ity and -ness , these don’t carry much semantic weight of their own,
aside from what comes with turning adjectives into nouns that mean
something like ‘the abstract quality of X’, where X is the base adjective.
Affixes like these are sometimes called transpositional affixes , meaning
that their primary function is to change the category of their base with-
out adding any extra meaning.
Contrast these, however, with affixes like those in (15) :
(15) a. -ee employee, recruitee, deportee, inductee
b. -less shoeless, treeless, rainless, supperless
c. re- reheat, reread, rewash
These affixes seem to have more semantic meat on their bones, so to
speak: -ee on a verb indicates a person who undergoes an action; -less
means something like ‘without’; and re- means something like ‘again’.
Languages frequently have affixes (or other morphological processes, as
we’ll see in Chapter 5 ) that fall into common semantic categories. Among
those categories are:
• personal or participant affixes: These are affixes that create ‘people nouns’ either from verbs or from nouns. Among the personal affixes
in English are the suffix -er which forms agent nouns (the ‘doer’ of
can be attached in a recursive fashion: convene → convention →
conventional → conventionalize → conventionalization .
First draw a word tree for conventionalization . Then see if you can
find other bases on which you can attach these suffixes recursively.
What is the most complex word you can create from a single base
that still makes sense to you? Are there any limits to the complexity
of words derived in this way?