MANDELBAUM AND POMERANTZ

CHAPTER 9 What Drives Social Action?

Jenny Mandelbaum Rutgets University

Anita Pomerantz Temple University

Conversation analysts aim to make sense of sense-making-to explicate the met hods through which everyday interactants produce discourse. This involves the close examination of tape-recorded, transcribed everyday conversations. Some of the topics studied by conversation analysts include how interactants take and give up turns (Sacks, Schegloff, & Jefferson, 1974), orient to sequences (Schegloff &Sacks, 1973), repair their own and others’ talk (Schegloff, Jefferson, & Sacks, 1977), issue invitations (Drew, 1984). seek information (Pomerantz, 1988), and tell about their troubles (Jefferson, 1980). ‘

Throughout these and other studies, we (as conversation analysts) are funda- mentally interested in participants’ concerns, orientations, and enterprises. At the same time, however, we have resisted addressing the question of intention. We often have written about participants’ orientations and concerns in almost behavioristic ways, giving discourse features as evidence of proposed orienta- tions and concerns. We have often proposed actors to be concerned about discourse features, a s opposed to being concerned about pursuing actions. Traditionally conversation analysts have described participants’ concerns in terms of their orientations to features of conversation. For instance, participants a r e portrayed as being concerned about whether or not a particular turn is to be understood to be the reason for the call, or whether and how the conversation is to be closed. and so forth (Schegloff, 1986; Schegloff & Sacks, 1973). These orientations are formulated as participants’ answers to the question, “Why that now?”-why is he or’she saying that thing a t this particular point?

Whereas conversation-analytic studies have tended to downplay or be inex-

 

 

152 MANDELBAUM AND POMERANTZ

plicit about the role of intentions, studies in speech communication have fre- quently unquestioningly treated interaction as wholly intenlional and goal- driven. As Craig (1 986, p. 257) has noted, tlie assumption that lluman behavior is goal-oriented is pervasive in many fields. Much of the work on goals in the speech communication field seems to be influenced by cognitive science models of action (e.g., Schank & Abelson, 1977). This work implicitly. and sometimes explicitly, takes the position that discourse is constructed in such a way as to enable interactants to achieve goals. I f it is a routine goal, actors will have a familiar script to play out in order to achieve the goal. I f not, a plan will be formulated for engaging in behavior that will lead to the satisfaction of the goal. According to this view. interaction is seen to be designed by a pre-existing goal. (For instance, cf. Rule & Bisanz, 1987; Canary, Cunningham, & Cody, 1988; Dillard, 1990, re: compliance-gaining goals. See Manusov, 1989. for a useful review.) Actions that a r e not in concert with that goal a r e sometimes described as “digressions” (Jacobs. Jackson, Hall, & Stearns, this volume).

Conversation analysis’s tendency to disattend or be inexplicit about partici- pants’ concerns, and speech communication’s tendency to regard all of social action a s goal-driven, suggest the need for a closer look at what drives social action, if indeed “driving” is the appropriate metaphor. Conversation analysis’s particular strength is its ability to identify details of social action by looking closely a t interaction. In this chapter we address the question of participants’ intentions in social action from a (our) conversation-analytic perspective. Drawing inferences from analysis of a fragment of conversation, we propose that. rather than generally being planfully driven by goals, .social action some- times is shaped by various types of concerns. We also note, however, that there a r e taken-for-granted features of interaction, and some more formal properties of it. that influence how social action is organized. In this chapter, we show how a close look suggests that social action is worked out between participants i n a n ongoing way, rather than driven by goals.

In order to answer the question, “What drives social action?”, we discuss a telephone conversation in which it is rather clear what the participants were trying to achieve. One participant was asking for help, and the other was declining t o help. This conversation provides us with materials for illustrating the distinctions we wish to make. Whereas some discourse choices were respon- sive to primary, multiple and contingent, or prerequisite concerns, other dis- course choices were influenced by taken-for-granted considerations or by other properties of social interaction. Below we clarify the proposed three concerns and two other influences by showing how they are manifested in conversation. Before turning to the telephone call, we briefly outline some of the issues raised by these distinctions.

First, we emphasize that we are dealing with inferredconcerns. Our data are the details of interaction. This approach does not permit us to determine what a n a r t i c i ~ a n t “actually” is concerned about. Indeed, we take it that such informa-

tion is currently very hard to obtain. Even self-reports of “actual” concerns a r e mediated by the situation in which they a r e solicited and may simply provide an account of what a participant remembers himself or herself to have been concerned about when prompted to think about it by a researcher. We inferred the mental states of the interlocutors from patterns in their interaction.

Second, level of awareness or consciousness of concerns is another cognitive matter to which we have no direct access. As noted above, when we propose that someone had a concern, we are involved in a process of inference. Sirni- larly, we inferred probable consciousness or absence of it from various features of the participants’ behavior. Although these parlicipants made their concerns apparent for one another and acted in ways that indicated that they wereat least sbhewhat aware of what they were achieving, we mark as problematic the answers to (a) how “aware” or “conscious” participants are, and@) the affects of their awareness (or lack of it) on their actions.

The following telephone call’ provides us with data for exploring an answer to, “What drives social action?”

O Ring

1 Sheila: Hello?

2 Rorlny: ‘lo Sheila,

I Ronny: ((7’s) Rjonny.

5 Sheila: Hi Ro~iny.

G Ronny: Guess what.hh

7 Sheila: What.

8 Ronny: .hh My ca:r is sta::lled.

9 (0.2)

10 Ronny: (‘n) I’m u p here in the Gle117

1 1 Sheila: 011::.

12 Ronny: h h h

13 Ronny: A:nd.lrh (0.2) 1 don’ know if it’s: po:ssible, but

14 .Ill111 see I heveta open up the ba:nk.hh

‘We wish lo thank Manny Scheglolf lor providing the data and for his contributions to theanalysis. We are also ~ r a t e l u l for his feedback on a late draft of this chapter. See appendix for transcription symbols.

 

 

154 MANDELBAUM AND POMERANTZ

16 Ronny: a:t uh: (.) in Brentwood?hh=

17 Sheila: =Yeah:- en I know you want- (.) en I whoa-

18 (.) en I would, but- except I’ve gotta leave

19 in about five min(h)utes. =

20 Ronny: [=Okay then I gotta call somebody else.right away.

21. Sheila: [(hheh)

23 Ronny: Okay?=

24 Sheila: =Okay [Ron 1

25 Ronny: [Thanksla lot.= Bye-.

26 Sheila: Bye:.

We offer five distinctions to help us better understand what drives the social action in this fragment of talk. These include primary concerns, rnultlple and contingent concerns, prerequisite concerns, taken-for-granted sustained achievements, and unmindful actions.

PRIHARY CONCERNS

We infer that Ronny’s primary concern here was to get help (most likely a ride) and that, in order to satisfy this concern, he called Sheila. The inference that this was Ronny’s primary concern is based on the following features of his talk. The call was organized around getting help. It was his first and only official business after identification was achieved. Ronny’s talk was limited to what was neces- sary for determining whether or not Sheila could help him: He identified the answerer as Sheila and himself to Sheila (lines 2-5), he indicated he had news to tell (line 6): he reported a problem that Sheila might have helped him to solve (lines 8, 10, 13-14, 16), and he closed the call immediately upon learning that Sheila could or would not help him out (line 20).

These details suggest that this was a single-purpose call. One could hypothe- size that there was an additional motive to the request. In the way that friend- ships may be tested with requests (Baxter & Wilmot, 1984), Ronny might have been testing whether Sheila was prepared to put herself out for him in time of need.2 w e do not know enough about the doing of secret tests to say whether or

‘We are differentiating Ronny’s making the request to test Sheila’s friendship from an effect that compliance with or relusal lo a request may have on a relationship. More specifically, although

9. WHAT DRIVES SOCIAL ACTION? 155

not this is likely. We see rlotlling in the discourse, however, to indicate that Ronny was concerned with testing the relationship.

MULTIPLE A N D CONTINGENT CONCERNS

In writing of “dialectical goals,” Craig (1986) pointed out that “people often face the need to do more than one thing a t the same time” @. 264). For example, In asking for someone’s help, participants often a r e concerned both with getting the help they seek and with not imposing on the other more than is necessary. Likewise, in declining to help friends, participants often are concerned both with not committing themselves to perform the help-tasks and with not engendering negative reactions.

We propose that both Ronny and Sheila were dealing with multiple concerns. More specifically, we propose that, when Ronny and Sheila enacted their primary concerns (i.e., getting help and declining to give help, respectively), there were additional concerns attached to those enactments. Next, we describe in detail how Ronny asked for help from Sheila in a way that minimized the degree to which he was imposing on her. This concern not to impose can be seen a s an additional concern that came about contingently, that is, upon finding that he needed to turn to someone for help. Sheila declined to help Ronny while displaying a sympathetic and friendly attitude toward his asking. This concern to be sympathetic and friendly also can be seen a s a contingent concern that came into being when Sheila chose not to help Ronny.

A characteristic of Rontly’s talk that makes available his contingent concern is its neutrality with respect to what he wanted. He began with “Guess what,” which indicated news but did not suggest whether the news was good or bad. He told his problem, without stating the upshot of it: “My ca:r is sta::lled . . . (‘n) I’m up here in the Glen? . . . see I haveta open up the ba:nk.hh . . . a:t uh: (.) in Rrentwood?hh.” Thus. Sheila was put in a position of “inferring” why he was reporting the situation to her. By describing a problem without making a n explicit request, Ronny gave Sheila a chance either to volunteer to help or to treat what he said as news. Sheila treated it as news in line 11 by indicating, with her “Oh::.,” that she had just been informed by what he said (see Heritage, 1984, re: change of state tokens). In reporting an event that can be heard a s a “trouble” or “problem,” Ronny most likely was counting on Sheila’s understanding of problems as needing solutions. In telling her the trouble or problem, he made available to her that she might help him, but he did not directly indicate anaction .

Sheila’s not having given help (or giving help had she done so) may or may not have had consequences for how Ronny viewed their friendship, that is quite separate lrom whether he made the rrn~lmt a s a Imt of her Irie~~dship.

 

 

9. WI-1AT DRIVES SOCIAL AC’KION? 157 156 MANDELBAUM AND POMERANTZ

that he would have liked Sheila to take in response to his p r ~ b l e m . ~ Thus Ronny gave Sheila an opportunity to offer the help or not, rather than putting her in a position of having to respond yes or no to an explicit request.

In addition to not making an explicit request for help. Ronny gave Sheila an “out.” In saying. “A:nd.hh (0.2) 1 don’ know i f it’s: po:ssible” (line 13), Ronny was displaying a n awareness that Sheila may have to turn him down. Ronny offered a basis that Sheila might have for not helping him that minimized the poter~tial offensiveness of her not helping him out. He proposed not that she would not want to but that she may not be able to, that is, that it would not be her choice. Hence he was making it easier for Sheila to not help, should that be the option she was selecting.

We a r e arguing, then, that through the way that Ronny constructed his talk, he showed Sheila that he had concerns that were attached to getting help. The way that he constructed his talk shows that he was also concerned with not imposing on Sheila. In reporting his problem without officially requesting Iiclp, Ronny enabled Sheila to volunteer to help him. In naming an inoffensive basis for her not helping him, he may have made it easier for her to decline to help. Thus, he attempted to actualize his primary concern in a way that was sensitive to his concern of not imposing on her.

Our account of what motivates Ronny here is based in details intrinsic to the talk, as is usual in conversation-analytic inquiry. ltis also possible to posit a more social psychological explanation for the phenomenon. As the following social psychological explanation indicates, though, it may be difficult to ground this explanation in the details of the talk.

Rawlins (1983a, 1983b) proposed a “dialectic” of friendship, in which friends a r e torn between the contradictory impulses of the freedom to be independent and the freedom to be dependent. Rawlins says. “While each person is free to pursue individual goals and interests separate from the other and without the friend’s interference o r help, each retains the liberty to call on the other for assistance, should it be necessary” (1983a, pp. 259-260).

In friendships, tensions often arise between wanting to help a friend, on the one hand, and wanting to pursue one’s own concerns, on the other. In asking a favor of a friend, interactants often a r e sensitive to this dilemma and attempt to minimize the expected inconvenience, avoid having the friend sacrifice per- sonal priorities, and/or insure that the friend perceives that h e o r she can say no. Ronny’s concern for getting help (his primary concern) carries with it a contin- gent concern of not overly imposing on Sheila.

Ronny again displayed this contingent concern in dealing with Sheila’s

‘See Drew. 1984. for a similar method used wilh respect lo making and lurning down ir~vilations. Drew noted that. when a speaker offers n report and leaves Ihe recipient lo draw the upsl~ot of tlic report. this is a device a spenker may use lo avoid making “rejecting” the talk’s ofliclal business.

declination to help. In lines 17-19, Sheila “anticipated” what Ronny was calling for and offered a reasoll for not helping him:

17 Sheila: =Yeah:- ell I know you want- (.)en 1 whoa-

18 (.) en I would, but- except I’ve golta leave

1 $1 in about five tnin(11)utes. =

In response to her giving a reason, lionny right away accepted her refusal. He did not question or challenge her reason. He did not plead, cajole, threaten, promise, or make any appeals designed to make her change her mind. Rather, he immediately accepted the reason and announced his intention to solve the problem another way:

2 0 ~ o n n y : [=Okay Ihen 1 gotta call somebody else.right away.

Ronny’s abandoning his attempt to get help from Sheila may have been related to his not wanting to put pressure on her to do something that she did not want to do. This concern might have emerged in that he was attempting to solicit Sheila’s help, where Sheila’s giving help may involve some degree of sacrifice on her part.

To reiterate, Ronny’s alertness to the possibility of imposing was suggested in the ways in which he made the request in the firs1 place and dealt with her response to it. He presented the matter in a way that allowed her to volunteer, and, once she indicated a problenl with giving the help that she inferred he wanted, he was very quick to accept her position.

Deriving our observations from this conversation we have argued that a favor asker may orient to the presumed imposition or cost of the recipient’s compli- ance by providing a choice and giving a legitimate out for the recipient. In addition, a favor asker may orient to the presumed imposition in deciding whom to ask. If one imposes to some degree on the selected target when soliciting help, then one selecls a target where that imposition is more or lessappropriate to the relationship. Ronny selected Sheila as a target for his request. When Sheila indicated unwillingness to help, Ronny quickly moved to report that he would select another target. ‘The multiple concerns of getting help while not “overly” imposing would seem to affect both the selection of the target and the manner in which help is sought.

Similarly, we infer that Sheila had contingent concerns in turning down Ronny’s request for help. Although Sheila clearly was concerned with pursuing her own activities, she turned him down in a way that showed friendliness toward him and a sympathetic attitude toward his problem. In lines 18-19, she turned dowri liis inexplicit request i l l a way that claimed a willingness to have done tlle favor but for circumstances:

 

 

158 MANDELBAUM AND POMERANT? 9. WHAT DRIVES SOCIAL ACTION7 159

18 Sheila: . . . en I would, but- except I’ve gotta leave 19 In about five rnin(h)utes. =

Sheila cojoined a claim of willingness (“en 1 would”) with a claim of uncontrol- lable circumstances (“I’ve golta leave in about live min(h)utes0 [emphasis a d d ed]). With this format, she treated his request as legitimate, that is, as one that was appropriate for him to askof her. In other words, in her refusal sl~e.in~plicitly claims that she was an appropriate target for the request, despite the refusal. Additionally, she may have displayed a sympathetic attitude toward his request by producing the refusal with disfluency, that is, behaving in a way that was interpretable as a show of discomfort a t turning him down.

Both Ronny’s and Sheila’s enactments show that contingent concerns accom- panied their primary concerns. Faced with the prospect of “needing to get help” or “turning down a request.” interactants may experience additional concerns, such as not imposing or offending the cointeractant. These additional concerns shape the way the actions are carried out.

A qualification is necessary here: We have called “not imposing” and “main- taining a friendship” contingent concerns because they manifested themselves in the enactments of Ronny’s getting help and Sheila’s declining to help, respec- tively. Presumably, a contingent concern in one circumstance may be a primary concern in another.

PREREQUISITE CONCERNS

Assume that an actor has a primary concern and engages in some interaction in an effort to satisfy that concern. Some of the behaviors displayed a r e shaped by his or her attempting to satisfy the primary concern (e.g., getting help). There also a r e behaviors that a r e shaped by contingent concerns (e.g., not overly imposing). Still other behaviors are products of yet another kind of concern: concerns that emerge by virtue of getting into the position of attempting to satisfy the primary concern. We call these prerequisite concerns.

A prerequisite concern is one that is a prerequisite to achieving the business of the conversation. For instance, prerequisite to Ronny’s getting help from Sheila is for Ronny to reach her. We assume that Ronny was oriented to whether Sheila was home or not, because his being able to reach her was a prerequisite for his seeking help from her. We d o not know whether Ronny presu~ned that Sheila would be home, thought it likely, or was indeed concerned about catching her. In the discourse that we have from the start of the phone call, we see no display of this concern.

We offer another example of a prerequisite concern. In order for Ronny to try to get Sheila’s help, he had to know that she knew that he was the caller. Hadshe

shown that she recognized his voice, he would have known that she knew his identity. As it turned out, she did not show that she knew who was calling (line 3):

O Ring

I Sheila: Hello?

2 Ronny: ‘lo Sheila,

In line 3, she confirmed her own identity without offering to identify the caller. In response tosheila’s just confirming her own identity, Ronny identified himself (line’ 4):

2 Ronny: ‘lo Sheila.

3 Sheila: Yea[:h 1

4 Ronny: [(‘t’s) Klol~ny.

5 Sheila: I l i Ronny.

Ronny’s specific concern to be recognized emerged after Sheila’s behavior in line 3 suggested that she may not have recognized him. Although in some phone calls the interactants unproblematically recognize each other’s voice, in other phone calls, interactants nlay become concerned with being recognized and/or recognizing the other by voice (Schegloff, 1979, 1986). In this case, we suggest that Ronny’s concern about whether Sheila recognized him may have emerged when her talk exhibited some doubt about it.

The two prerequisite concerns that we discussed are Ronny’s possible con- cern with whether Sheila was at home and Ronny’s emergent concern with whether Sheila recognized him as the caller. Both a r e offered as concerns that Ronny may have dealt with before being able to seek help from Sheila.

You didn't find what you were looking for? Upload your specific requirements now and relax as your preferred tutor delivers a top quality customized paper

Order Now